Skip to main content
Log in

Judgments of others’ heights are biased toward the height of the perceiver

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined how observers use one aspect of their own morphology, height, when judging the physical characteristics of other people. To address this, participants judged the heights of people as they walked past. We tested the hypothesis that differences between participant and target height account for systematic patterns of variability and bias in height estimation. Height estimate error and error variability increased as the difference between participant height and target height increased, suggesting that estimates are scaled to observers’ heights. Furthermore, participants’ height estimates were biased toward two standards, demonstrating classic category effects. First, estimates were biased toward participants’ own heights. Second, participants biased height estimates toward the average height of the target distribution. These results support past research on using both the body and categorical information to estimate target properties but extend to real-world situations involving interactions with moving people, such as height judgments provided during eyewitness testimony.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This result appears to be inconsistent with previous research, since a prior study demonstrated bias away from observers’ own heights (Twedt et al. 2012). These studies differed in many ways that could have led to disparate results. The present work relied on perceptual judgments, whereas the earlier work examined judgments from memory. In addition, the present study tested absolute judgments of height, whereas the earlier study tested relative judgments between two targets with a dichotomous shorter than or taller than judgment, and was not explicitly designed to measure bias. Finally, the present study used real people as targets rather than inanimate objects. Further research is needed to understand how different forms of bias arise from such differences in methodology. The social categorization literature provides many examples of how contrast and assimilation effects are dependent on a diverse set of factors, including context (Stapel & Suls, 2007).

References

  • Anastasi, J. S., & Rhodes, M. G. (2005). An own-age bias in face recognition for children and older adults. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 1043–1047.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertamini, M., Yang, T. L., & Proffitt, D. R. (1998). Relative size perception at a distance is best at eye level. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 673–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M. (2005). Standards and expectancies: Contrast and assimilation in judgments of self and others. Essays in social psychology. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Duguid, M. M., & Goncalo, J. A. (2012). Living large: The powerful overestimate their own height. Psychological Science, 23, 36–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, H. (2005). Heteroscedasticity and complex variation. In B. S. Everitt & D. Howell (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Statistics in Behavioral Science. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 220–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linkenauger, S. A., Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2011). Taking a hands-on approach: Apparent grasping ability scales the perception of object size. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1432–1441.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 3–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ogden, C. L., Fryar, C. D., Carroll, M. D., Flegal, K. M. (2004). Mean body weight, height, and body mass index, United States 1960-2002 (pp. 1-17). Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics.

  • Pozzulo, J. D., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (1998). Identification accuracy of children versus adults: A meta-analysis. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 549–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt, D. R., & Linkenauger, S. A. (2013). Perception viewed as a phenotypic expression. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action Science: Foundations of an Emerging Discipline (pp. 171–199). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, Norbert and Herbert Bless (1992), “Constructing reality and its alternatives: An inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment,” In L.L. Martin and A. Tesser (Eds.), The Construction of Social Judgments, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Sedgwick, A. (1973). The visible horizon: A potential source of visual information for the perception of size and distance (Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 34, 1301.

  • Stapel, D. A., & Suls, J. (Eds.). (2007). Assimilation and Contrast in Social Psychology. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Twedt, E., Crawford, L. E., & Proffitt, D. R. (2012). Memory for target height is scaled to observer height. Memory and Cognition, 40, 339–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, W. H., Jr. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683–703.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Malpass, R. S., Lindsay, R. C. L., Fisher, R. P., Turtle, J. W., & Fulero, S. M. (2000). From the lab to the police station: A successful application of eyewitness research. American Psychologist, 55, 581–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 277–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wraga, M. (1999). The role of eye height in perceiving affordances and object dimensions. Perception and Psychophysics, 61, 490–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wraga, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (2000). Mapping the sone of eye-height utility for seated and standing observers. Perception, 29, 1361–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, D. B., & Stroud, J. N. (2002). Age differences in lineup identification accuracy: People are better with their own age. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 641–654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elyssa Twedt.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Twedt, E., Crawford, L.E. & Proffitt, D.R. Judgments of others’ heights are biased toward the height of the perceiver. Psychon Bull Rev 22, 566–571 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0689-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0689-z

Keywords

Navigation